The greatest hurdle in writing about works informed by turn-of-the-last-century science is communicating how unscientific it was. Unlike medievalists, who have the luxury of discussing barnacle geese and self-castrating beavers, I address works with the patina of reputable science—the toys and methods are patently modern, the problem lies in the conclusions drawn with their aid. Individually, each of these conclusions is merely incorrect; combined, they form the keystone for many a house of scientific horror, each built to the idiosyncratic specifications of its inmate-owner.
Compounding my difficulty is that from the street, these monstrosities are indistinguishable from the stolid brownstones flanking them on either side. Only when you open the front door and step into a mud-walled bathroom in which a stove sits beneath a sign reading "FOR SHITTING APRICOTS" do you sense something is amiss.
In short, convincing you that a conventional-sounding statement about social evolution is no more rational than a foyer dedicated to disposing peaches ain't the easiest stunt to pull. Here's my malformed attempt:
Early in ["The Descent of Man"], [Edith] Wharton mentions [Professor Linyard's] authorship of “Ethical Reactions of the Infusoria” and “The Unconscious Cerebration of Amoeba” (314). These microscopic animals are not, however, his area of expertise: “On the structure and habits of a certain class of coleoptera [beetles],” Wharton writes, “he was the most distinguished living authority” (318). His interest in infusoria indicates the speculative—almost unscientific, but certainly unprofessional—cant of his thought.
That he extends his investigation into the ethics of infusorial reaction points to a metaphysical tendency extant prior to writing [his popular mockery of metaphysically informed "science"] The Vital Thing. Infusoria are so named because they appear in infusions of decaying organic matter. Throughout the latter half of the nineteenth century, debates as to the classification of infusoria persisted. “Though the term Infusoria has usually been applied to all the Protozoa provided with cilia or flagella,” declares A.S. Packard in 1875, “it is not restricted to the highest [Ciliata] division of the Protozoa” (87).
In the same issue of The American Naturalist in which Packard’s essay appears, noted evolutionist T.H. Huxley places them within the category of “Endoplastica,” which “while not forsaking the general type of the single cell, attain a considerable complexity of organization” (66). Infusoria are thus single-celled animals with uncharacteristic complexity; unless, that is, they are not animals at all. David S. Kellicott, delivering the keynote address at the annual meeting of the American Society of Microscopists, notes that it is without question that “these [infusoria] teeming in the hay infusion are alive,” then asks, “but why relegate them to the animal kingdom rather than to the vegetable?” (14).
He then reminds his fellow microscopists that no lesser light than Ernst Haeckel proposed a third kingdom, Protista, in which to include animals with chlorophyll-bearing bodies. Straddling the fence between animal and vegetable, infusoria and related protozoa occupied a central place in early debates about the evolution of mind. The eminent neo-Lamarckian E.D. Cope argues that
It is evident…that education commences low in the scale, since some of the acts of Infusoria indicate an adaptation of means to ends which cannot be supposed to be possible to a totally new experience. The discharge of weapon-like cilia of the Dinidium at its prey would indicate that the animal knew the effect of the act from past experience, and anticipated that food would be secured in this way from its success in previous performances of the kind. (“The Evolution of Mind,” 904)
Linking consciousness to adaptations like “weapon-like cilia” is a conventionally Lamarckian move, for “specialization of structure means specialization of function; and specialization of function means accomplished education” (903). Specialization is the result of adaptations acquired over time, Cope argues, and the mechanism of adaptation for Lamarckians is force of will; therefore, if infusoria possess specialized structures—and they do—then Cope must “[believe] in the presence of consciousness in Protozoa” (903).
That it would have been possible to believe that microscopic organisms occupying the neutral slot between animal and vegetable possess rudimentary consciousness is enough to open infusoria to endless speculation of the sort Linyard produces in The Vital Thing; that they were considered immortal by many opened more pseudoscientific avenues still.
R. von Lendenfeld outlines the argument for infusoria immortality in “The Undying Germ-Plasm and the Immortal Soul” (1891). The theory originates with the fierce neo-Darwinian, vehement anti-Lamarckian August Weismann who, as von Lendenfeld summarizes, argued:
All unicellular beings, such as the Protozoa and the simpler Algae, Fungi, &c., reproduce themselves by means of simple fission. The mother-organism may split into two similar halves, as the Amoeba does, or, as is more common in the lowest unicellular plants, it may divide into a great number of small spores. In these processes it often happens that the whole body of the mother, the entire cell, may resolve itself into two or more children; at times, however, a small portion of the mother-cell remains unused…From this it follows that these unicellular beings are immortal. (92)
Despite being only arguably animal, infusoria possess consciousness and are immortal, which facts forced scientists and sociologists to consider the possibility that “colonies” of infusoria were but a single, immortal individual. Even if it is, the cooperation between individual infusoria are considered the basis of what in 1902 Arthur Allin calls “sociality.”
Following Herbert Spencer’s analogy of society as an organism, Allin notes that “Selfishness necessarily generates altruism. The chalk cliffs of infusoria are the result of the individualistic action of each of the infusoria, the infusorium being typical of egoism” (81). The infusoria thus represented the lowest form of life in which social behaviors equivalent to those of human society could be observed. As Henry Fairfield Osborn observed in 1892, “the most impressive truth issuing from…recent researches in evolution and heredity is the uniformity of life-processes throughout the whole scale of life from Infusoria to man” (668).
Does that sound suitably insane?
Would Wharton have realized the depths into which she's dropped her reader, or is she just echoing the 'state of the art' of the day?
As to the excerpt itself, I think the ocassional "which we know now is absurd" (or some variant thereof; you'll think of something) might help.
Posted by: Jonathan Dresner | Sunday, 22 July 2007 at 12:29 AM
I was going to write that I found it insane, but maybe I wasn't the ideal reader because maybe I knew something about evolution, etc.
Then my eye chanced upon “The Undying Germ-Plasm and the Immortal Soul” and it became apparent that this will sound insane to everyone.
Posted by: Justin | Sunday, 22 July 2007 at 12:53 AM
Do colonies of infusoria form chalk cliffs? (Not quite sure I'm following.) But if so, this makes me think of the white cliffs of Dover. Is this guy sending a subliminal message (to me, here, now) that in fact England is the highpoint of social evolution, the most altruistic of nations? This is not crazy, it's brilliant. Brilliant!
Alternatively, the White Cliffs of Infusoria could be a landmark somewhere in the Wacky World of Victorian Science.
Posted by: John Attridge | Sunday, 22 July 2007 at 03:43 AM
Hearing 'immortality' and 'Ernst Haeckel' in the same breath reminds me that this kind of talk isn't limited to 19th C. 'protista' and 'germ-plasm.' You do know that Haeckel had a hand in the definition of the concept stem cell, a definition which still has resonance and relevance today?
I think this article does a reasonable job of reviewing early terminology and exploration of SCs.
The idea of 'immortality' is important here as well -- stem cells were originally seen as defined by their ability to proliferate and self-regenerate, making them effectively immortal.
But it's only been in the last few years that we've been able to actually unravel what that means, in molecular and genetic terms.
Anyway. It's not like these ideas are just the 'bad science' of the 19th century. The strains and descendants of this kind of thought have seeped into the models and ideas of a lot of modern scientists, too.
Posted by: son1 | Sunday, 22 July 2007 at 08:55 AM
The question, gentlemen, is not whether infusoria have an Immortal Soul but to what purposes these "forms of rudimentary consciousness" and social behaviors have been put. I ask you, are these germ-plasms Church of England or are they, as their teeming collectivity seems to indicate, (shudder) Papists? And how shall we send a force to claim them for the glory of Her Majesty, considering their very small size?
Posted by: Sisyphus | Sunday, 22 July 2007 at 09:47 AM
The science of today is the mockery of tomorrow. (When we're lucky, anyway -- I occasionally get very panicked emails about spermatorrhea....)
My picks to click on Top of the Mocks: memes, subcellular material viewed as itty-bitty neoliberal capitalists, and the neuro-imaging fad: "Culturally influenced behavior causes brain activity!! It's outta control!" But I've seen some pretty absurd stuff about consciousness lately, too.
Posted by: Ray Davis | Sunday, 22 July 2007 at 10:29 AM
I'm not sure which parts of the paragraph you're actually picking out as bad science. I'm going to number some possibilities to try to get more clarity:
1. The idea that purposeful-seeming behavior implies consciousness. This, I agree, is an interesting Lamarckian pseudo-scientific bit as written in the example from E.D. Cope, with the discharge of cilia implying the learning of effects from experience.
2. The idea that organisms that reproduce by fission can be immortal. This idea is, as far as I can tell, still in use. If you agree that a single-celled organism has no individual consciousness, then really all it has is unbroken metabolic activity and an unchanged genetic code to say that it has not yet died. So it seems like a single-celled organism that reproduces by fission could be said to be immortal until a time is reached at which all of its descendents are either dead or their genetic material has mutated. This isn't immortality in the way that a person's soul is sometimes believed to be immortal, but it isn't mortality in the sense that a person suffers from either.
3. The idea that organisms with the same DNA form a single super-organism. The idea that a colony is best thought of as an individual seems pretty common in neo-Darwinian writing. And I've seen popular biological articles about the largest living organism being some form of fungus that spreads underground for miles with unchanging DNA; clearly that's on a similar principle.
Posted by: Rich Puchalsky | Sunday, 22 July 2007 at 07:42 PM
4. Conclusions about other organisms, and particularly the evolution of other organisms, can be extended straightforwardly to humans, and human society: individualism leads to altruism.
Posted by: John Attridge | Monday, 23 July 2007 at 02:59 AM
Haven't had the chance to say so, but I do appreciate all the comments. I've taken them all into account -- as well as few others, emailed to me -- but haven't had time to respond, so busy am I finishing the chapter itself. Needless to say, if this ever makes it into a book*, you'll be thanked.
*Please please please let it be me let it be me.
Posted by: SEK | Tuesday, 24 July 2007 at 09:59 PM
Oddly, I study Barnacle Geese and Self castrating beavers. Not as I think you seem to be suggesting from a post-modern perspective. I use the conventional approach of cultural evolution myself although I am not a scientist. As I don't know anyone else working on such themes who exactly is incorrect and shitting for apricots? The remark with regard to reinforcing social prejudice I find particularly ironic (though not I would note in a P. modernist sense) As most of what I do is informed by an evolutionary perspective seems a bit weird.
I thought science worked on the basis of evidence rather than assumption? From peer review of published completed papers.
Posted by: Jeb | Thursday, 18 February 2010 at 01:04 PM
p.s with regard to social prejudice and other assumptions, I would draw this article to youre attention by W.F Bynum on A.O. Lovejoy.
http://articles.adsabs.harvard.edu/cgi-bin/nph-iarticle_query?1975HisSc..13....1B&data_type=PDF_HIGH&whole_paper=YES&type=PRINTER&filetype=.pdf
Like Lovejoy I have an interest in Lord Monboddo, which is where I first came across A.O.L. Monboddo of course had a particular interest in the beaver. His work of course was not scientific but I think that is clearly demonstrated when it comes to discussing his very unusual taste in source material.
Posted by: Jeb | Thursday, 18 February 2010 at 01:24 PM